November 17, 2017 — If you’re like many Americans, you drink fluoridated water without questioning its safety or efficacy. For decades municipalities have been adding fluoride to water supplies to reduce the incidence of tooth decay at the advice of numerous highly credentialed entities, including the World Health Organization, the U.S. Public Health Service, the American Dental Association and the American Academy of Pediatrics.
But in late 2016, a coalition of activist groups — Food and Water Watch, The American Academy of Environmental Medicine, Fluoride Action Network, the International Academy of Oral Medicine and Toxicology, the Organic Consumers Association, and Moms Against Fluoridation — together with private citizens from around the United States petitioned the U.S. Environmental Protection Agency (EPA) to ban the addition of fluoride to water supplies, citing research pointing to health risks linked to exposure to fluoride.
What exactly are the benefits of fluoridating municipal water supplies? What are the risks? And how does a conscientious society balance the two? These questions are finding new urgency as a growing body of research calls into question its safety.
Fluoride’s Benefits
The practice of adding fluoride to drinking water began in the 1940s after dentists observed that children living in communities with naturally high levels of fluoride had fewer cavities than those who didn’t. Several communities began adding fluoride to municipal water supplies as a public health measure to lessen the prevalence of dental decay. Studies revealed that cavity rates in those communities were lower than in comparison communities that did not fluoridate, and increasing numbers of municipalities decided to fluoridate in ensuing decades. Rates of dental decay continued to drop. Today, more than 200 million Americans live in communities with fluoridated water. (Most, but not all, major U.S. cities fluoridate.)
However, some argue that other factors during this period, such as advances in dental care and widespread use of fluoridated toothpaste, are likely behind these declines. In fact, European countries that never fluoridated have seen very similar drops in cavity rates.
Howard Pollick, professor at the University of California, San Francisco School of Dentistry and spokesperson on fluoridation for the American Dental Association, says it has become “harder and harder to separate the benefit of water fluoridation in light of the use of fluoride products” such as toothpaste and fluoride varnishes. Designing a study that can tell us with certainty that fluoridated water confers benefits above and beyond what we derive from other sources of fluoride and dental care like sealants, he says, is “very difficult.”
In an effort to quantify the benefits of fluoridating water, the Iowa Fluoride Study has followed 570 participants from infancy to young adulthood. Researchers have found that children living in communities that fluoridate their water don’t have statistically significantly fewer cavities than those in unfluoridated communities.
Steven Levy, who directs the study, says that the demographics of study participants, who report brushing regularly and receiving consistent dental care, likely affect results. For those who lack access to fluoridated dental products and preventive dental care, Levy speculates that fluoridated water’s impact on dental health may be considerably greater. In fact, the U.S. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention estimates that fluoridated water reduces cavities by 25 percent which includes low-income populations who Levy says generally have much higher rates of tooth decay. Critics point out that this reduction amounts to perhaps one cavity per person and argue that these benefits to teeth — minimal for those who brush regularly with fluoride toothpaste and see a dentist regularly — must be weighed against the risks to other parts of the body.
Potential for Harm?
The EPA in February 2017 denied the 2016 petition on the grounds that it lacked “scientifically defensible” proof of neurotoxic harm caused by fluoridated water. But Kathleen Thiessen, senior scientist at Oak Ridge Center for Risk Analysis who has three decades experience assessing fluoride’s risk, maintains that the agency dismissed many high-quality studies. Further, Thiessen says the sheer number of studies, despite their limitations, requires taking the possibility of harm from fluoride more seriously. And Harvard adjunct professor of environmental health Philippe Grandjean, who has spent his career investigating the effects of environmental pollutants on children’s development, says evidence suggesting fluoride’s potential for harm warrants reconsidering its addition to drinking water.
Indeed, over the past two decades, numerous studies have begun to challenge assumptions about the safety of ingesting fluoride.
A 2014 paper on developmental neurotoxins Grandjean and colleague Philip Landrigan of the Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai in New York published in The Lancet included fluoride in a list of chemicals with developmental neurotoxic effects. In 2012 Grandjean worked on a systematic review of studies from China showing lower IQ in areas with naturally high levels of fluoride. Though these studies had some design issues, Grandjean contends that when more than 20 discrete studies point to a similar effect, they should receive serious consideration rather than be dismissed for their limitations. If anything, he says, the imprecision in these studies likely “led to an underestimation of the effects” of fluoride on neurodevelopment.
A study published in Environmental Health Perspectives in September 2017 sought to address the shortcomings of previous studies. Looking at nearly 300 mother-child pairs, researchers analyzed samples of pregnant mothers’ urine archived in the Mexican Early Life Exposures in Mexico to Environmental Toxicants (ELEMENT) project. The study measured maternal urinary fluoride and cognitive performance in children and found significantly lower cognitive performance in children whose mothers had higher levels of fluoride. Howard Hu, founding dean of the Dalla Lana School of Public Health at the University of Toronto and co-author of the study, says that results may underpredict the relationship between fluoride and IQ.
Research currently underway aims to clarify the relationship between fluoridated water and urinary concentrations of fluoride. ADA’s Pollick has reviewed studies on fluoride’s health effects and says that the dental community has taken them into consideration. He reports that at present “the evidence points to its being safe” and notes that we lack information about urinary levels in Americans.
Research currently underway aims to clarify the relationship between fluoridated water and urinary concentrations of fluoride. The Canadian “Maternal-Infant Research on Environmental Chemicals” (MIREC) study is examining urinary fluoride levels of women during pregnancy and subsequent behavior of their children. The MIREC study took samples from 1,960 pregnant women in 10 Canadian cities, half of which fluoridate drinking water. Preliminary findings suggest urine levels in women from fluoridated communities in the Canadian study are comparable to those found in the Mexico research, according to study director Christine Till, associate professor of clinical neuropsychology at Toronto’s York University.
Linda Birnbaum, director of the U.S. National Toxicology Program, oversaw a 2016 review of animal studies on fluoride that she says determined the available evidence inconclusive. The NTP is currently reviewing human epidemiological data, including the recent ELEMENT study. Its findings, says Birnbaum, help confirm conclusions drawn from the Chinese studies on fluoride and IQ and show that smaller increases in fluoride concentrations are associated with impaired cognitive performance. While she calls it “an important study,” she also notes that it requires replication.
Researchers face numerous difficulties, Birnbaum says, including quantifying how much fluoride people ingest, since “it’s everywhere now,” including in prepared foods and beverages, such as soup, soda and coffee made with water sourced from fluoridated community water supplies that reach people living in communities without fluoridated water. Since everyone is likely consuming some fluoride, finding a comparison group that doesn’t is nearly impossible. What additional research will hopefully help us do, says Birnbaum, is “understand differential susceptibility — are there some people because of genetics or past history who are more sensitive [to fluoride] than others?”
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is another area of scrutiny. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is another area of scrutiny. A study published in Environmental Health in 2015 found that every 1 percent increase in the prevalence of water fluoridation in 1992 was associated with 67,000–131,000 additional cases of ADHD between 2003 and 2011. Till, who co-authored the study, says that such observational studies cannot prove causation. She and other researchers are using the Mexican ELEMENT and Canadian MIREC study data to better understand possible relationships between fluoride exposure and ADHD.
Because fluoride can interfere with absorption of iodine and affect thyroid function, some research has looked into the possible links between drinking fluoridated water and the prevalence of thyroid disorders. A 2015 study in the Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health looking at the association between drinking water fluoride and hypothyroidism in two regions of the United Kingdom found the prevalence of hypothyroidism prevalence was almost two times higher in the fluoridated area as in the unfluoridated one. Critics have challenged the study design and argued that other confounding factors should have been accounted for. A 2017 Canadian report using data from the Canadian Health Measures Survey (CHMS) found no association between fluoride exposure and incidence of reported thyroid disorders in fluoridated communities. Other research has found some correlation between fluoride exposure and elevated or lowered levels of different thyroid hormones.
Burden of Proof
The coalition that petitioned the EPA in 2016 has since filed a complaint with the U.S. district court in northern California calling for a new review of the evidence. If the court decides that water fluoridation indeed poses an “unreasonable risk” of harm, it can require the EPA to ban the addition of fluoride to drinking water supplies. The EPA has filed a motion to dismiss the case. Plaintiffs have requested a trial they expect to take place in 2018.
Challenging the status quo on chemical safety has become increasingly difficult, says Bruce Lanphear, a public health researcher at Simon Fraser University whose work focuses on disease and disability prevention. The evidence required to alter policy, he says, has risen dramatically in the last several decades, thanks in part to powerful industry lobbies.
Lanphear urges public health officials to reconsider long-held assumptions as science yields results that call them into question. Lanphear urges public health officials to reconsider long-held assumptions as science yields results that call them into question. Scientists used to believe there was a safe threshold of exposure for virtually all chemicals. In the case of compounds such as lead and benzene, Lanphear says, “the evidence suggests that assumption is no longer valid,” with “proportionately greater harms at lower levels” of exposure. Further, he explains, our bodies are “bombarded” by a huge range of chemicals that may collectively predispose us to greater risk of disease. While each individual toxicant may have a limited impact on health, when combined they could greatly increase the risk of negative health effects.
At present, many focused on preventing tooth decay are not persuaded of fluoride’s health risks, while fluoridation opponents argue sufficient evidence exists of fluoride’s negative effects on the body that far outweigh its marginal benefit to teeth. As research on fluoride continues to clarify its biological effects and litigation pushes government to reconsider policy, governing bodies, communities, and individuals may find themselves grappling anew with how to weigh the potential benefits of fluoride with its possible harms to human health.
Editor’s note: Susannah Shmurak produced this feature as a participant in the Ensia Mentor Program.
Ensia shares solutions-focused stories free of charge through our online magazine and partner media. That means audiences around the world have ready access to stories that can — and do — help them shape a better future. If you value our work, please show your support today.
Yes, I'll support Ensia!
It is important to ask --- exactly why should an ADULT be sentenced to take this toxic chemical, fluoride, in every glass of water every day of life?
Fluoridation results in slow poisoning over a lifetime which causes premature ageing, thyroid damage, dental fluorosis, lowered IQ, ADHD, brittle bones (broken hips & arthritis), kidney damage, cancer and other health dangers.
Read this excellent book, "The Case Against Fluoride" authored by three scientists, one an M.D. It contains over 1200 scientific references, over 80 pages.
So, while 74% of the U.S. is forced to drink fluoridate water, only 5% of the world and only 3% of Europe fluoridate their water. China and Japan have rejected it many years ago.
1. “Food and Water Watch, The American Academy of Environmental Medicine, Fluoride Action Network, the International Academy of Oral Medicine and Toxicology, the Organic Consumers Association, and Moms Against Fluoridation”......are all fringe activist groups notorious for disseminating misinformation about fluoridation, and which have no credibility within respected science and healthcare. Placing them in the same sentence with highly respected healthcare organizations such as the WHO, USPHS, ADA, and AAP conveys an implied sense of equality. There is not.
While over 100 of the most highly respected healthcare and healthcare-related organizations in the world publicly recognize the public health benefit of fluoridation there is not one credible organization which opposes it.
As noted deeper in this article, the EPA summarily rejected the petition by these activist groups. In its 40 page rejection, citing facts and evidence, EPA reviewers systematically dismantled all arguments of the petitioners, including detailed explanations of the invalidity, irrelevance, and/or misrepresentation by petitioners, of the numerous studies presented as evidence by the petitioners.
Kathleen Thiessen is a long time, outspoken fluoridation opponent. Her confirmation bias against fluoridation was demonstrated in her endorsement of a 2013 study by William Hirzy, the current paid lobbyist for the New York antifluoridationist faction, "Fluoride Action Network". Hirzy used data from his study to petition the EPA to recommend cessation of use of HFA as a fluoridating substance. When EPA reviewers looked at Hirzy's data, however, they quickly determined that Hirzy had made a 70-fold error in his calculations. After correcting for these errors, the reviewers found Hirzy's data to demonstrate the exact opposite of what he had concluded. Upon learning of his error and rejection of his petition, Hirzy stated that he was "embarrassed", as well he should have been.
Prior to the EPA review of Hirzy's data, Thiessen had been asked to comment on his study. Her response?
"I think this is a reasonable study, and that they haven't inflated anything," said Kathleen Thiessen, a senior scientist at SENES Oak Ridge Inc., a health and environmental risk assessment company."
-----http://news.yahoo.com/arsenic-drinking-water-costly-change-could-lower-levels-103332699.html
Thiessen’s opinion on the EPA rejection of the latest antifluoridationist petition, is obviously too biased to have any credibility.
The entire EPA rejection document may be viewed on the Federal Register:
https://www.federalregister.gov/documents/2017/02/27/2017-03829/fluoride-chemicals-in-drinking-water-tsca-section-21-petition-reasons-for-agency-response
As was their right, the petitioners filed an appeal, not a “complaint”, of the EPA rejection in federal court. The appeal was a conglomeration of unsubstantiated anecdotal stories from antifluoridationists, with the same arguments and evidence which the EPA reviewers had already completely refuted. Given that the court will rely upon the same body of peer-reviewed scientific evidence and understanding of the fallacies of the studies presented as evidence by the petitioners, an assumption that there will appear some alternative set of facts and evidence magically supporting the arguments of the petitioners, is obviously ludicrous.
2. Countless peer-reviewed scientific studies through the decades have clearly demonstrated the effectiveness of fluoridation in the prevention of significant amounts of dental decay in entire populations. These studies are current through the present and are readily accessible.
The attempts by fluoridation opponents to minimize the often lifelong devastating effects of untreated dental decay with terms such “perhaps one cavity per person” negate their claims that fluoridation is ineffective and demonstrate a dangerous naivety of dental disease. In actuality, dental decay is a dangerous bacterial infection that occurs in close proximity to the brain with a direct pathway to the rest of the body via the bloodstream. When left untreated as they often are, this “perhaps one cavity” claimed by antifluoridationists can, and does, lead to lifetimes of extreme pain, black discoloration of some or all of the teeth, development of serious medical conditions, and life-threatening infection. Such a “perhaps one cavity” in but one tooth has led to death directly resultant of that dental infection.
3. The similarity in dental decay incidence of European countries frequently cited by antifluoridationists is based on a misrepresentation of raw WHO data by personnel of the Fluoride Action Network, to imply that fluoridation is ineffective. Aside from that misrepresentation, attempting to assess but one dental decay preventive measure based on raw data which controlled for none of the myriad variables involved in dental disease is ludicrous.
An excellent explanation of the fallacy of the country comparison argument is provided by New Zealand chemist, Ken Perrott:
https://openparachute.wordpress.com/2015/08/12/fluoridation-connetts-naive-used-of-who-data-debunked/
4. The Iowa Fluoride Study was not “an effort to quantify the benefits of fluoridating water”. It was a study of the effects on dental caries and fluorosis from both dietary and non-dietary sources of fluoride. Neither did it find that “children living in communities that fluoridate their water don’t have statistically significantly fewer cavities than those in unfluoridated communities.”
In response to a request for comment on “FAN” founder Paul Connett’s claim in item #3 of his “10 Ugly Facts” that the Iowa Study “found no relation between tooth decay and the amount of fluoride swallowed”, Dr. Steven Levy stated the following:
1) We looked at total F intake from almost all sources (water, beverages, selected foods that absorb water, dietary F supplements, dentifrice)(they acknowledge this ok in their point #3)
2) But we did not say that we "found no relation between tooth decay and the amount of fluoride swallowed", but that it is very complicated--e.g., those with caries but not mild dental fluorosis tended to have lower F intake than the other 3 sub-groups
3) And in many other published articles and abstracts as well as unpublished data, we consistently see ~14-20% less decay among those in F areas, across exams at several ages (for prevalence at 5, 9, 13 and 17 years and incidence across 4-year intervals) --even after adjusting for all that we can (brushing with F dentifrice, SES,
dietary exposures, F supplements, etc.)
-Steven M. Levy, DDS, MPH
Wright-Bush-Shreves Endowed Professor of Research
Department of Preventive & Community Dentistry
University of Iowa
College of Dentistry
5. Given that there is no valid, peer-reviewed scientific evidence to claims of “harm” from optimally fluoridated water there is no foundation for the claim that benefits must be weighed against risks to other parts of the body. There are no such “risks”.
6. In regard to the infamous “27 Chineses studies” of the 2011 meta-analysis of Grandjean and Choi, there were far more than just “some design issues”. These were studies dug out of obscure Chinese journals by researchers Grandjean and Choi. These studies were of the effects of high levels of fluoride (as high as 11.5 ppm) in the well-water of various Chinese, Mongolian, and Iranian villages.
By the admission of Grandjean and Choi, themselves, these studies had key information missing, inadequate control for confounders, and questionable methodologies. These 27 studies were so seriously flawed that Grandjean and Choi were led to issue a public statement in March, 2012 that the studies should not be used to judge water fluoridation in the US. This obviously has not stopped antifluoridationists from doing so anyway.
"These results do not allow us to make any judgment regarding possible levels of risk at levels of exposure typical for water fluoridation in the U.S. On the other hand, neither can it be concluded that no risk is present. We therefore recommend further research to clarify what role fluoride exposure levels may play in possible adverse effects on brain development, so that future risk assessments can properly take into regard this possible hazard."
--Anna Choi, research scientist in the Department of Environmental Health at HSPH, lead author, and Philippe Grandjean, adjunct professor of environmental health at HSPH, senior author.
https://cdn1.sph.harvard.edu/wp-content/uploads/sites/21/2012/07/Media-Statement_Fluoride-9-12-12-Revised2.pdf
7. Grandjean’s personal classification of fluoride as neurotoxin as noted the “Lancet” article was based solely on his and Choi’s meta-analysis of the 27 Chinese studies. See item #6 above.
A refute of Grandjean’s opinions may be found in a subsequent issue of the “Lancet”:
http://www.thelancet.com/journals/laneur/article/PIIS1474-4422(14)70119-X/fulltext
8. The 2017 Mexican ELEMENT study of Bashash, et al. had stringent limitations, as are clearly stated in the study itself. These limitations included:
A. A lack of data on urinary fluoride content of pregnant women living in fluoridated areas of the United States. Fluoridation opponents seek to do what the authors, themselves, did not do....compare urinary fluoride content of one population with non-existent data of another.
“Finally, our ability to extrapolate our results to how exposures may impact on the general population is limited given the lack of data on fluoride pharmacokinetics during pregnancy. There are no reference values for urinary fluoride in pregnant women in the United States” 1
B. Insufficient information to rule out significant confounders.
“Nevertheless, it is not possible to entirely rule out residual confounding or in the population as a whole (that might have been detected had we had full data on larger sample sizes) or bias (should the subpopulations that had the data for analysis have a different fluoride–cognition relationship than those participants who were excluded from the analyses).”
C. There was no significant effect noted by fluoride exposure on children after birth.
“Finally, in models that focused on the cross-sectional relationship between children’s exposure to fluoride (reflected by their specific gravity–adjusted urinary fluoride levels) and IQ score and that contained the main covariates of interest, there was not a clear, statistically significant association between contemporaneous children’s urinary fluoride (CUFsg) and IQ either unadjusted or adjusting for MUFcr.” 1
D. Evidence suggested that IQ effects were noted only with prenatal urinary content above 0.8 mg/L.
“The associations with GCI appeared to be linear across the range of prenatal exposures, but there was some evidence that associations with IQ may have been limited to exposures above 0.8 mg/L.” 1
E. Results would need to be replicated by independent researchers.
“Our findings must be confirmed in other study populations, and additional research is needed to determine how the urine fluoride concentrations measured in our study population are related to fluoride exposures resulting from both intentional supplementation and environmental contamination.” 1
F. “Other limitations include the lack of information about iodine in salt, which could modify associations between fluoride and cognition; the lack of data on fluoride content in water given that determination of fluoride content is not reported as part of the water quality monitoring programs in Mexico; and the lack of information on other environmental neurotoxicants such as arsenic.” 1
G. Bashash, et al., utilized the “spot” collection method of urine collection, rater than the more
accurate 24 hour method.
As noted by Deena Thomas, second author of Bashash, et al:
“The spot urine samples we measured cannot account for diurnal variations in urinary fluoride. Previous studies report fluctuations in concentration by time-of-day42 which means that the fluoride levels in our samples could be influenced by the time-of-collection or by diet.” 2
Additionally, in her doctoral thesis, Deena Thomas, using the exact data as did Bashash, reported on the effects of prenatal fluoride on children aged 1, 2, and 3. She found no significant effect on these children. Bashash, chose to omit these findings, and instead began their IQ assessment in children 4 years of age.
Additionally, in a September, 2017 email to Dr. Johnny Johnson, Jr, Dr. Angeles Mier Martinez, one of the lead researchers in the Bashash, et al. Mexican study, relayed:
1) As an individual, I am happy to go on the record to say that I continue to support water fluoridation”
2) If I were pregnant today I would consume fluoridated water, and that if I lived in Mexico I would limit my salt intake.”
3) “I am involved in this research because I am committed to contribute to the science to ensure fluoridation is safe for all.”
E. Angeles Martinez Mier, DDS, MSD, PhD
Cariology, Operative Dentistry and Dental Public Health
Indiana University School of Dentistry
415 Lansing Street
Indianapolis IN 46202
9. In a response to the 2016 findings of the NTP study of the effect of fluoride on rats, the US CDC stated:
“Above all, we want to assure you that the NTP report does not change this recommendation, and CDC maintains its confidence that optimally fluoridated drinking water is a safe and effective tool to prevent tooth decay in children and adults.”
“The objective of the NTP evaluation was to conduct a systematic review of existing animal studies to develop NTP level-of-evidence conclusions about whether fluoride exposure is associated with impairment in learning and memory. The review included 32 studies that exposed laboratory animals to fluoride during development or adulthood and then assessed them for neurobehavioral effects based on their responses to tests of learning and memory. It is important to note that almost all of the studies they reviewed used exposure levels that far exceeded the PHS recommended level for human consumption in public water systems (0.7 parts per million); many of the studies assessed exposures at 50 or 100 times those levels or more.”
“NTP used the GRADE system for rating the confidence in the body of evidence. Researchers concluded that there was a low level-of-evidence for learning and memory effects on rats or mice treated during gestation through adulthood. “Low level-of-evidence” reflects NTP’s limited confidence in the apparent relationship between fluoride and learning and memory and a probability that the true effect may be substantially different from the apparent relationship.”
“Among rats and mice exposed shortly after weaning or in adulthood, researchers found a moderate level-of-evidence to support an effect on learning and memory from the Morris water maze test, and a low level-of-evidence to support an effect on learning and memory from the T-maze, Y-maze, and other tests. The “moderate level-of-evidence” reflects a possibility that the true effect may be reflected in the apparent relationship, but the researchers’ confidence in this conclusion is constrained by weakness present in the included studies.”
http://americanfluoridationsociety.org/wp-content/uploads/2016/07/CDC-letter-re-NTP_CDC-Dear-Colleage-Letter_July-2016.doc
10. The Malin/Till ADHD study published in Grandjean’s online publication has been so widely discredited in the scientific literature for its.....
a. Inadequate control for confounders
b. Poor methodology
C. Reaching a conclusion not supported by the peer-reviewed science
........that it is a wonder that anyone still attempts to mislead the public with it.
The inadequate control for confounders by Malin/Till was clearly demonstrated by the 2015 Huber, et al. study which, using the exact same data, concluded the reported cases of ADHD to be correlated with the elevation level at which the children resided, not fluoridated water.
—-J Atten Disord. 2015 Mar 25. pii: 1087054715577137.
Association Between Altitude and Regional Variation of ADHD in Youth.
Huber RS, Kim TS, Kim N, Kuykendall MD, Sherwood SN, Renshaw PF, Kondo DG.
An example of the critiques of Malin/Till:
"It's an ecological study design with 51 observations (50 states & DC), and is not appropriate to test a hypothesis. ADHD prevalence was based on self-reported data, and hence had a potential of misclassification of disorder status. State-wide fluoridation measures were used. Individuals' exposure to fluoridation were not measured. Due to ecological assessment of exposure to fluoride in drinking water and the use of prevalence data of self-reported ADHD and water fluoridation from different years, the findings are at high risk for ecological fallacy. Authors did not adjust for important confounders (smoking, low birth weight, age, sex etc.). Moreover, authors' poor literature review and skewed interpretation of literature concerning fluoride and neurodevelomental defects may have introduced bias.
------http://www.fluoridescience.org/articles/exposure-to-fluoridated-water-and-attention-deficit-hyperactivity-disorder-prevalence-among-children-and-adolescents-in-the-united-states-an-ecological-association/#sthash.tQUCMKJi.dpuf
11. Similarly the 2015 UK hypothyroid study by long time fluoridation opponent Stephen Peckham, has also been widely discredited in the scientific literature for its inadequate control for confounders, poor methodology, and reaching a conclusion not supported by the peer-reviewed science.
One such critique:
“In summary, this study [Peckham] is an ecologic one that has several significant flaws, making it almost meaningless with regard to assessing any possible association between water fluoridation and hypothyroidism. As such, this study provides no evidence of a causal relationship between water fluoride concentration and hypothyroidism.”
No Evidence Supports the Claim That Water Fluoridation Causes Hypothyroidism
J Evid Base Dent Pract 2015;15:137-139
J.J. Warren, Maria C.P. Saraiva
12. Lanphear’s personal opinions are based on assumptions that the claims of antifluoridationists are credible. As I have clearly demonstrated, they are not.
As they have since the beginning of the initiative 7 decades ago, antifluoridationists are calling for the sudden cessation of the public health initiative of water fluoridation, an initiative which has provided a very valuable disease prevention benefit to hundreds of millions of individuals of all ages for the past 72 years, with no proven adverse effects. As such, it is their responsibility to provide valid evidence to support their request. Thus far they have provided nothing but unsubstantiated claims, misinformation, misrepresented science, unfounded speculation, and personal opinions.
Steven D. Slott, DDS
Communications Officer
American Fluoridation Society
The fluoridation agents added to drinking water include sodium hexafluorosilicate and fluorosilicic acid. Both sodium hexafluorosilicate and fluorosilicic acid are listed in Section 8(b) of the Toxic Substances Control Act (TSCA; chemical inventory section). The chemicals were once used as pesticides, but all pesticide products containing these substances had their registrations canceled or they were discontinued by the early 1990s.
One look at the CDC’s MSDS sheet on fluorosilicic acid (URL: https://www.cdc.gov/niosh/ipcsneng/neng1233.html) would compel anyone with common sense to ask, “Why would any rational person add this stuff to drinking water?” Or consider the 2001 Review of Toxicological Literature, published by the National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences (URL: https://ntp.niehs.nih.gov/ntp/htdocs/chem_background/exsumpdf/fluorosilicates_508.pdf), with its numerous confirmed exposures of the two chemicals by humans and the harmful effects they endured. Exposure to the chemicals by workers in the water treatment field is rarely even considered, which is shameful in of itself.
The bottom line: if the practice of adding fluoride has the slightest hint of being potentially harmful, it should be stopped immediately. Topical application of fluoride can be carried out by those who believe it will benefit them.
Finally, I would like to say to Steven Slott (Nov. 17, 2017 comment) that calling the International Academy of Oral Medicine and Toxicology (IAOMT) “a fringe activist group notorious for disseminating misinformation about fluoridation and have no credibility within respected science and healthcare” is a false and libelous. The IAOMT members are a global network of dentists, health professionals, and scientists who research the biocompatibility of dental products. The non-profit Academy’s members are allied professionals dedicated to providing scientific resources to support the highest levels of integrity and safety in healthcare. Despite their outrageous characterization by people like Mr. Slott, IAOMT members have selflessly dedicated enormous amounts of time and effort to advance their mission of protecting public health and the environment since 1984.
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=FH_y3Cw8MVg&t=241s
WATCH BY SELECT TOPICS
FLUORIDE : The Bizarre History 1/20
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=qYR57ZYvRuU
FLUORIDE : Cosmetic Effects 2/20
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=sgFcnN3ArOo
FLUORIDE : The Air We Breathe 3/20
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=kpUq4-F4Y74
FLUORIDE : The Great Fluoride Scam 4/20
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=bi-7ds-ErH8
FLUORIDE : The Experts at NAS/NRC 5/20
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=fs5_deDuu2Y
FLUORIDE : Legal Issues 6/20
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=BODrm_V98iY
FLUORIDE : Funky Teeth 7/20
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=AULGk6V2-9A
FLUORIDE : Thyroid Hormones 8/20
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=7XWREK-X-aY
FLUORIDE : Intelligence 9/20
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=6U9l4YolGJA
FLUORIDE : UNICEF 10/20
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=A8AhMQ4rrr4
FLUORIDE : How Fluoridation Got Started 11/20
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=pEhQUeBQhfA
FLUORIDE : ISFR Fluoride Journal 12/20
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=YBRzUfBSkqg
FLUORIDE : Brain Development 13/20
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=w5iobtWOjyI
FLUORIDE : The United States Economy 14/20
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=Gr6DbDSMVUk
FLUORIDE : C&EN Special Report 15/20
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=slX2JdCXqIs
FLUORIDE : The Natural Resources Defense Council 16/20
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=UoTR6GCBvP8
FLUORIDE : The Phosphate Connection 17/20
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=zwzWr7c_zEQ
FLUORIDE : EPA Regulations 18/20
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=Ejc_mS0aTSI
FLUORIDE : The Aging Factor 19/20
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=2XzeYupNS-k
FLUORIDE : Conclusion 20/20
http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=iK4WiFZSvCE